Walk Backward to Better Memory

Anika Huizinga / UnsplashSource: Anika Huizinga / Unsplash

Whenever we’re remembering some incident from our past, it’s not uncommon for us to say we’re “thinking back” on it, figuratively attributing spatial characteristics to non-spatial time. We know that space and time are separate entities, but when we think of our relationship to time we often picture ourselves travelling along a timeline with the future extending in front of us and the past stretching out behind us. We look “forward” to the future and “back” the past. While thinking of time in terms of spatial dimensions is, of course, purely figurative, a recent study in England suggests that thinking of time in terms of physical space offers unexpected and potentially useful benefits when it comes to remembering the past. Through a series of experiments designed to test the effect of backwards travel through space on recall of events occurring in the recent past, Aksentijevik et al. demonstrated that thinking “back”—or more specifically thinking “backward”--to the past can improve our memory of what we experienced there.

In a series of six experiments, participants were presented with either a video of a staged crime, a word list, or a set of pictures, and later tested on the accuracy of their recall of the video, words, and pictures. Between the presentation and test, the participants engaged in one of three different activities designed to measure the effect of backward motion, forward motion, or no motion on their ability to answer questions about the video or recall the words and pictures. In a “physical motion” activity, participants either walked forward toward a target, walked backward toward a target, or stood still for two minutes—approximately the amount of time it would have taken them to receive instructions and walk to the target. In an “imaginary motion” activity, participants closed their eyes and imagined themselves walking either forward or backward to a target, or stood still for two minutes. In an “optic flow” activity, participants watched a video filmed from the back platform of a moving train. Some participants watched a forward version of the video, which moved backwards from the observers’ perspective, while others watched a reverse version of the video, appearing to move forward form the observers’ perspective. A control group of participants stood still for two minutes.

Don't use technology as an emotional pacifier. Media can be very effective in keeping kids calm and quiet, but it should not be the only way they learn to calm down. Children need to be taught how to identify and handle strong emotions, come up with activities to manage boredom, or calm down through breathing, talking about ways to solve the problem, and finding other strategies for channeling emotions.

Throughout the series of experiments, participants in the backward motion conditions—whether real, imaginary, or virtual--exhibited better recall than the participants in either the forward motion or no motion conditions. This correlation between backward motion and improved recall suggests “some form of temporal indexing” through which related memories “are ordered on the subjective time line and stored in a temporal sequence or a time-indexed spatial cluster.” The mental experience of moving backwards through space appeared to carry the participants’ minds backward along that subjective timeline toward the point at which the remembered information was encoded, thus improving their recall.

Aksentijevic et al. have labelled the observed effect of “motion-induced mental time travel” on memory as the “Mnemonic Time Travel Effect.” While their research on the phenomenon is admittedly in its early stages, they believe that it shows promise for potentially developing “motion-based mnemonic aids" which, among other possible applications, could prove beneficial to the elderly and people with dementia. Whatever the practical applications of the “Mnemonic Time Travel Effect” may turn out to be, the results of the present study offer compelling evidence for the dynamic relation of memory to the entirety of human experience. Rather than being “an enclosed domain whose task is to retain information and make it available,” memory is but a component of a larger “embodied system which brings together perception, thinking and action.”

Gossip about your kids. Fact: What we overhear is far more potent than what we are told directly. Make praise more effective by letting your child "catch" you whispering a compliment about him to Grandma, Dad, or even his teddy.

The embodied nature of memory blurs the line between space and time, and, where descriptions of memory are concerned, between literal and figurative language. The timeline upon which we traverse the course of our lives is thus a physical as well as a mental construct, and a trip down memory lane as much an occasion for walking shoes as for a shoe box full of old photographs.